Освободи голову. Экспресс-метод для сохранения ясности ума, улучшения концентрации и развития креативности - Фолькер Буш


Освободи голову. Экспресс-метод для сохранения ясности ума, улучшения концентрации и развития креативности читать книгу онлайн
Оставайтесь продуктивными и здоровыми в повседневной цифровой жизни!
Но как? Невролог и психиатр Фолькер Буш представляет проверенную концепцию улучшения концентрации, креативности и внимания – а, значит, и снижения стресса.
Современные помехи: информация постоянно бомбардирует нас и засоряет головы, мессенджеры отвлекают от реальной жизни, а быстрый темп жизни делает наше мышление поверхностным. Мы чувствуем себя перегруженными и истощенными, становимся беспокойными и совершаем ошибки. Наш фокус смещен с того, что действительно важно!
Здесь читатели узнают, как:
– концентрироваться на главном среди обилия информации;
– улучшить память;
– обрести спокойствие в суматошной жизни;
– сохранить тихие моменты, во время которых они смогут развивать свои творческие идеи.
157. Perham, N. and J. Vizard, Can preference for background music mediate the irrelevant sound effect? Applied Cognitive Psychology, 2011. 25(4): p. 625–631.
158. Reaves, S., et al., Turn Off the Music! Music Impairs Visual Associative Memory Performance in Older Adults. Gerontologist, 2016. 56(3): p. 569 – 77.
159. Wang, Z. and J. M. Tchernev, The» Myth «of Media Multitasking: Reciprocal Dynamics of Media Multitasking, Personal Needs, and Gratifications. Journal of Communication, 2012. 62(3): p. 493–513.
160. Yeykelis, L., J. J. Cummings, and B. Reeves, Multitasking on a Single Device: Arousal and the Frequency, Anticipation, and Prediction of Switching Between Media Content on a Computer. Journal of Communication, 2014. 64(1): p. 167–192.
161. Group, T. R., Email Statistics Report, 2020–2024–Executive Summary. 2020.
162. Billieux, J., Problematic use of the mobile phone: A literature review and a pathways model. Current Psychiatry Reviews, 2012. 8(4): p. 299–307.
163. Fried, I., et al., Electric current stimulates laughter. Nature, 1998. 391(6668): p. 650–650.
164. Lachmann, O., Die Bekenntnisse des heiligen Augustinus. Reclams Universal Bibliothek, ed. Reclam. Vol. 2791/94a. 1888, Leipzig.
165. Valdez, P., Circadian Rhythms in Attention. The Yale journal of biology and medicine, 2019. 92(1): p. 81–92.
166. Currey, M. and M. Currey, Daily rituals: how artists work. 2013.
167. Backhaus, N., A. Tisch, and A. Wöhrmann, BAuA-Arbeitszeitbefragung: Vergleich 2015–2017, in Bundesanstalt für Arbeitsschutz und Arbeitsmedizin 2018, BaAuA, Editor. 2018.
168. Wendsche, J. and A. Lohmann-Haislah, Ein Scoping-Review zur Wirkung von Arbeitspausen auf die Gesundheit. 2016.
169. Bosch, C. and S. Sonnentag, Should I Take a Break? A Daily Reconstruction Study on Predicting Micro-Breaks at Work. International Journal of Stress Management, 2018. 26.
170. Tine, M., Acute aerobic exercise: an intervention for the selective visual attention and reading comprehension of low-income adolescents. Front Psychol, 2014. 5: p. 575.
171. Kang, S. and T. R. Kurtzberg, Reach for your cell phone at your own risk: The cognitive costs of media choice for breaks. J Behav Addict, 2019. 8(3): p. 395–403.
172. Ariga, A. and A. Lleras, Brief and rare mental» breaks «keep you focused: Deactivation and reactivation of task goals preempt vigilance decrements. Cognition, 2011. 118(3): p. 439–443.
173. Busch, V., et al., The effect of deep and slow breathing on pain perception, autonomic activity, and mood processing – an experimental study. Pain Med, 2012. 13(2): p. 215–28.
174. Ma, X., et al., The Effect of Diaphragmatic Breathing on Attention, Negative Affect and Stress in Healthy Adults. Frontiers in psychology, 2017. 8: p. 874–874.
175. Ulrich, R. S., View through a window may influence recovery from surgery. Science, 1984. 224(4647): p. 420–1.
176. Djernis, D., et al., A Systematic Review and Meta-Analysis of Nature-Based Mindfulness: Effects of Moving Mindfulness Training into an Outdoor Natural Setting. International journal of environmental research and public health, 2019. 16(17): p. 3202.
177. Berman, M. G., et al., Interacting with nature improves cognition and affect for individuals with depression. J Affect Disord, 2012. 140(3): p. 300–5.
178. Stenfors, C. U. D., et al., Positive Effects of Nature on Cognitive Performance Across Multiple Experiments: Test Order but Not Affect Modulates the Cognitive Effects. Frontiers in Psychology, 2019. 10(1413).
179. Kaplan, S., The restorative benefits of nature: Toward an integrative framework. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 1995. 15(3): p. 169–182.
180. Ohly, H., et al., Attention Restoration Theory: A systematic review of the attention restoration potential of exposure to natural environments. Journal of Toxicology and Environmental Health, Part B, 2016. 19(7): p. 305–343.
181. Lee, K. E., et al., 40-second green roof views sustain attention: The role of micro-breaks in attention restoration. Journal of Environmental Psychology, 2015. 42: p. 182–189.
182. Song, C., H. Ikei, and Y. Miyazaki, Physiological Effects of Visual Stimulation with Forest Imagery. International journal of environmental research and public health, 2018. 15(2): p. 213.
183. AL-Ayash, A., et al., The influence of color on student emotion, heart rate, and performance in learning environments. Color Research & Application, 2016. 41(2): p. 196–205.
184. Ueland, B., If You Want to Write. 1987: Graywolf Press.
185. Fink, A., et al., The Creative Brain: Investigation of Brain Activity During Creative Problem Solving by Means of EEG and fMRI. Human brain mapping, 2009. 30: p. 734–48.
186. Kleibeuker, S. W., et al., Prefrontal cortex involvement in creative problem solving in middle adolescence and adulthood. Developmental Cognitive Neuroscience, 2013. 5: p. 197–206.
187. GUS, S., Maßnahmen zur Gesundheitsförderung, Prävention und Rehabilitation mit Effektivitätsnachweisen. Fachbereich Gesundheitspsychologie in der Sektion GUS im Berufsverband der DPP (BDP), 2005.
188. Thoits, P.A., Stressors and Problem-Solving: The Individual as Psychological Activist. Journal of Health and Social Behavior, 1994. 35(2): p. 143–160.
189. Chinaveh, M., The Effectiveness of Problem-solving on Coping Skills and Psychological Adjustment. Procedia – Social and Behavioral Sciences, 2013. 84: p. 4–9.
190. Bucay, J. and S. von Harrach, Komm, ich erzähl dir eine Geschichte. 2011: FISCHER E-Books.
191. Oviedo-Trespalacios, O., et al., Problematic Use of Mobile Phones in Australia … Is It Getting Worse? Frontiers in Psychiatry, 2019. 10(105).
192. Statista. Anteil der Freizeit in der OECD nach Ländern. 2009; https:// de.statista.com/statistik/daten/studie/37072/umfrage/freizeitanteilan-einem-durchschnittlichen-tag-in-der-oecd-nach-laendern/.
193. Wiessner, P. W., Embers of society: Firelight talk among the Ju/’hoansi Bushmen. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2014. 111(39): p. 14027–14035.
194. Boyd, B., The evolution of stories: from mimesis to language, from fact to fiction. WIREs Cognitive Science, 2018. 9(1): p. e1444.
195. Killingsworth, M. A. and D. T. Gilbert, A Wandering Mind Is an Unhappy Mind. Science, 2010. 330(6006): p. 932–932.
196. Nightingale, A., Augustine on Extending Oneself to God through Intention in advance. Augustinian Studies, 2015. 46.
197. Teasdale, J. D., et al., Stimulus-independent thought depends on central executive resources. Mem Cognit, 1995. 23(5): p. 551–9.
198. Lagerfeld, K., Tagträumen ist vielleicht die wichtigste Arbeit in meinem Leben. Bild, 2012.
199. Raichle, M. E., et al., A default mode of brain function. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences
200. Beaty, R. E., et al., Creativity and the default network: A functional connectivity analysis of the creative brain at rest. Neuropsychologia, 2014. 64: p. 92–98.
201. Kühn, S., et al., The Importance of the Default Mode Network in Creativity – A Structural MRI Study. The Journal of Creative Behavior, 2014. 48.
202. Mooneyham, B. and J. Schooler, The Costs and Benefits of